from Books &
Reviews
"Finding a Needle in the Ocean"
from
Homosexuality and the Politics of
Truth
by Jeffrey Satinover, M.D.
Book Excerpt...
As you will recall, there are three disputed propositions
at the heart of the debate over homosexuality today:
- Homosexuality is normal;
- Homosexuality is innate, or inherited;
- Homosexuality is irreversible.
In this chapter and the three that follow we
will address the second point: "Is homosexuality innate and
inherited?" As we will, see the very way the question has been
framed by gay activism and its media promoters contributes
seriously to our confusion.
Defining Our Terms
Let us start by defining our terms. When we
analyze and discuss the causes of a given behavioral
trait, we find that each cause belongs to one or more of the
following categories:
Genetic
Genetic traits are those (like eye color, for
example) that are coded for us by genes. We can think
of each human gene as a book that provides a complex set of
instructions for the synthesis of a single protein. These
proteins are then responsible for forming and operating
everything else in the body.
The entire collection of genes that provides
codes for a human is vast. Therefore it is divided into
twenty-three pairs of matched, physically distinct structures
called chromosomes. We can think of them as matching
libraries that contain and catalogue two copies of every
required "book" (gene) in a specific order that does not vary
from person to person. Chromosome libraries exist in pairs
because each person actually has two instructional genes for
every protein, receiving one of every gene from his mother and
another from his father. The unvarying order in which the
genes are catalogued allows each one of the millions and
millions of genes to be matched to its proper companion during
reproduction.
Any genetic trait inherited from our parents
may be:
- Expressed, as when the gene that codes for it is
dominant and we have inherited at least one such gene from
each parent (brown eyes; brown is dominant); or as when the
gene that codes for it is recessive and we have inherited
the same gene from both parents (blue eyes; blue is
recessive).
- Not expressed, as when the gene that codes for it is
recessive and we have inherited that gene from only one
parent (brown eyes; blue is recessive and therefore not
expressed).
- Partially expressed, as when—whether the gene that codes
for it is dominantor recessive—one or more other genes or
other factors influences its expression. We may or may not
have some or all of these other genes, or we may be only
partially exposed to these other factors (green eyes).
Genetic traits are truly and directly
inherited. All traits with which we are born tend to be
put into this category, sometimes incorrectly, such as those
that are innate but not genetic.
Understanding behavioral traits
influenced by genetics becomes more complex. Unlike simple
traits, such as eye color, that are close to being programmed
by a single gene, most behavioral traits with a genetic
background are programmed by multiple genes. Because these
genes are rarely inherited together, their possible forms of
expression fall into a complex spectrum. Behavioral traits
that are influenced by genetics are therefore never eitherlor
conditions.
Innate
Some traits may be merely innate, meaning the
individual is born with them. But innate traits may be:
- Genetic, as outlined above; or
- Not genetic, but caused by intrauterine influences.
These are traits (such as the degree to which a fetus
develops masculine or feminine sexual characteristics) that
are influenced by various aspects of the environment in the
womb. Hormones, infections, exercise, general health, the
ingestion of licit or illicit drugs, and many other
variables influence this environment. Thus one may be born
with a trait that is innate, but not genetic.
Familial
Other causes of traits may be familial, meaning that
they tend to be shared by members of the same family. Familial
traits may be:
- Genetic. Because they have the same parents, brothers
and sisters are more likely to share a high percentage of
similar genes than would unrelated individuals.
- Innate, but not genetic. Sharing the same mother,
certain typical factors may remain constant or similar for
all children born to her. Examples include the effect of her
dietary habits on her unborn children, the fact that she
smokes, or her general health.
- Not innate, but environmental. To an extent greater than
between individuals from different families, individuals
raised in the same family share a similar environment. These
include the physical, emotional, and moral influences. Thus
family members may share some traits that are neither
genetic nor innate but that are nonetheless transmitted from
one generation to the next by influence.
Biological
Another term that may be used to describe a
trait is biological. A biological trait is rooted in an
organism's physiology, rather than its psychology. With
respect to behavioral traits, this distinction suggests a
dichotomy comparable to the difference between "hardware" and
"software" in the domain of computer science. Biological
traits may be:
- Genetic; or
- Innate but not genetic; or
- Environmental and familial but not innate (for example,
the effect of a virus that has taken root among the members
of a household); or
- Environmental and not familial and possibly innate but
maybe not (for example, the effect of a toxin in the
environment at large, depending on whether its baleful
influence is felt pre- or postnatally).
Environmental
Additionally, the cause of a trait may be
purely environmental but not biological, at least
insofar as we do not attend to the biological dimension.
Examples include the influence on behavior of the values,
standards, habits, economic status, and so on, of a family or
society.
Direct versus Indirect
Finally, any of these causes may be direct
or indirect. That is, the cause may:
- Lead directly to the trait. Whether we are speaking of
genetic or nongenetic, innate or noninnate, biological or
nonbiological influences, the cause may directly produce the
trait itself, as when genes cause blue eyes or when smoke
causes a cough.
- Lead indirectly to the trait. Because of what the
influence causes directly, the individual finds it desirable
to choose a particular trait. This is seen, for example,
when tall athletic individuals become basketball players or
when short athletic people become jockeys.
- Furthermore, all of these causes may combine and
influence one another in highly interdependent ways,
mutually influencing each other throughout a lifetime.
Behavioral traits, as opposed to simple, single-gene
physiologic traits such as eye-color, always interact in
this way.
In summary, the question concerning all
behavioral traits, such as homosexuality, cannot be "Is
such and such genetic?" Rather we must ask, "To what
extent, respectively, is such and such genetic and
nongenetic, innate and noninnate, familial and nonfamilial,
environmentally determined and not, direct and indirect? In
the course of development, when do which influences dominate
and how do their interactions affect one another?" We need to
keep this sobering caution in mind as we clarify what medical
science has and has not learned about the subject of
homosexuality.
Unanswered Questions
Most mental states, normal or not, have long
been presumed to be of psychological origin because we
have not been able to understand the biology. We simply did
not have the information or skills to intervene in a purported
disease of the brain whose primary manifestations were
psychological. But neuroscience research techniques have
proliferated. We now can dissect out at least some of the
specific mechanisms, down to the level of molecules, that play
a role in many conditions previously thought to be purely
psychological. Although this research has already produced
many dramatic benefits, we are far from having a precise
blueprint of the various causes of any psychiatric
condition.
Demonstrating that any behavioral state, let
alone one so complex, diverse in its manifestations, and
nuanced as homosexuality, is not only biological but
genetic is well beyond our present research capacity.
One psychiatric researcher who was tired of the overblown
claims of people trying to label everything as "genetic,"
calculated what would be required to confirm a behavioral
trait as genetic. He projected that "If the trait was 50
percent heritable and each family in the [initial] study had
ten members (4 grandparents, 2 parents and 4 children),
detecting one of the genes would require studying . . . 2000
people. Replicating that finding would require studying . . .
another 8000 people. To find and confirm each additional gene
(for a polygenic trait), researchers would need to go through
the whole business again. "Suddenly you're talking about tens
of thousands of people and years of work and millions of
dollars." No study of homosexuality has come even remotely
close to these requirements.
In the case of schizophrenia, for instance,
such research efforts have only now begun to yield somewhat
reliable results after over forty years of effort. But even
after so much research, the major questions - Mat causes
schizophrenia? How does this illness affect the nervous
system? What environmental cofactors are critical to its
appearance? What interventions might be curatives.— remain
almost entirely unanswered.
Different studies claim to show anywhere from
40 to 90 percent heritability for schizophrenia. Researchers
have made numerous claims to have found a meaningful "genetic
linkage" to a particular chromosome, only being forced to
retract them in every case.{1}
The vastly more complex problem of finding the genes
themselves or the specific DNA base pairs among the millions
on the chromosome has been compared to finding a needle, not
in a haystack, but in the ocean.
What We Can Say
In the case of homosexuality, only a handful
of barely adequate studies on a small number of people have
been conducted in the past few years. We will explore these
more fully in the chapters that follow. But first it is
important to lay out three important limitations that are
already beginning to emerge from this research. All are quite
consistent with what we already know about the biological and
genetic bases of other conditions.
First, like all complex behavioral and mental
states, homosexuality is multifactorial. It is neither
exclusively biological nor exclusively psychological but
results from an as-yet-difficult-to-quantitate mixture of
genetic factors, intrauterine influences (some innate to the
mother and thus present in every pregnancy, and others
incidental to a given pregnancy), postnatal environment (such
as parental, sibling, and cultural behavior), and a complex
series of repeatedly reinforced choices occurring at
critical phases in development.
Second, male and female homosexuality are
probably different conditions that arise from a different
composite of influences. Nonetheless, they have some
similarities.
Third, "homosexuality" is very poorly
defined. Our use of this one term creates the false impression
of a uniform "gay" or "lesbian" condition and culture. It
obscures the reality that what we are studying is a complex
set of variable mental, emotional, and behavioral states that
are caused by differing proportions of numerous influences.
Indeed, one of the chief characteristics of the gay lifestyle
is its efflorescence of styles and types of sexuality. Thus
many of the more careful researchers in the field, usually
nonactivist, refer to ''homosexucll~ties.''
Do Brain Differences Make a Difference?
The belief that homosexuality is "genetic"
tends to translate into a more positive attitude toward it.
Gay activists know this and research studies confirm it:
"To measure the relationship between beliefs
about the determinants of homosexual orientation and attitudes
toward homosexuals, we asked 745 respondents in four societies
about their beliefs concerning the origins of homosexual
orientation. Analysis indicated that subjects who believed
that homosexuals are "born that way" held significantly more
positive attitudes toward homosexuals than subjects who
believed that homosexuals choose to be that way and/or learn
to be that way{2}."
Similarly:
"One hundred and five . . . subjects . . .
were exposed to one of three treatment conditions.
Subjects in the experimental group read a
summary article of current research emphasizing a biological
component of homosexual orientation. Subjects in one control
group read a summary article of research focusing on the
absence of hormonal differences between homosexual and
heterosexual men. Subjects in another control group were not
exposed to either article. All subjects completed the Index of
Attitudes Toward Homosexuals. As predicted, subjects in the
experimental group had significantly lower{3}
scores than subjects in the control groups.{4}
This "public relations" effect has
precipitated a recent media outpouring on the biology and
genetics of homosexuality. Starting in 1991, media all across
the country have trumpeted the discovery of a series of
supposed brain differences between homosexuals and
heterosexuals. Commentators claim that these findings will
halt any remaining uncertainty that homosexuality is either a
choice or a consequence of factors in upbringing. In this
light, to continue supporting anything less than full
acceptance of homosexual behavior would be proof positive of
prejudicial hatred.
The outpouring began in August of 1991 when a
San Francisco neuroanatomist, Simon LeVay, published an
article in Science. It reported his finding that a
localized cluster (a "nucleus") of cells in the brains of
"homosexual" men was twice as large by volume on autopsy as in
"heterosexual" men.{5}
("Homosexual" and "heterosexual" are in quotations because in
this study the definitions of each were extremely imprecise,
nor was there any way of verifying sexual orientation as the
subjects were dead.)
But this was not the first such discovery.
One year before a group reported in Brain Research that
they had found a similar difference—in both volume and
number of cells—in a different brain nucleus.{6}
The media, however, did not report this first study because
Brain Research, unlike Science, is read only by
neuroscientists. And in contrast to journalists, the
neuroscientists understood the research and its limitations
and refrained from grand pronouncements.
The specifics of these findings are not as
important as realizing that unless group differences are
dramatic, individual studies of such differences mean almost
nothing. It would take hundreds, perhaps thousands, of such
studies before meaningful trends emerge. Thus it is wrong for
the media, or parties with vested interests, to argue the
significance of something so complex as human nature on the
basis of one or a handful of findings and then derive public
policy implications.
Furthermore, even if such brain differences
were convincingly demonstrated to be present, their
significance would be on a par with the discovery that
athletes have bigger muscles than nonathletes. For though a
genetic tendency toward larger muscles may make it easier to
become an athlete (and therefore such an individual will more
likely be one) becoming an athlete will certainly give one
bigger muscles. One researcher comments: "The brain's neural
networks reconfigure themselves in response to certain
experiences. One fascinating NIH study found that in people
reading Braille after becoming blind, the area of the brain
controlling the reading finger grew larger."{7}
Press accounts, in contrast, are often
written so as lead one to assume that brain differences must
be innate and unchangeable, especially differences in the
number of cells as contrasted with the simple volume occupied
by a collection of cells. We tend to think of the mind as
"software" and the brain as "hardware," the former plastic and
changeable, the latter fixed at birth. We have used this
analogy already to good advantage.
But the analogy breaks down at a certain
point. Various processes go on throughout life: the selective
death of brain cells in response to training or trauma, the
establishment of new connections between cells, dramatic
increases or decreases in the "thickness" of connections
between cells as a result of learning, the loss of
interneuronal connections through "pruning." Very unlike our
modem computers, the brain's software is its hardware.
We know from animal studies that early
experience and especially traumatic experience that especially
applies to the childhood histories of many homosexuals, alters
the brain and body in measurable ways. Thus infant monkeys who
are repeatedly and traumatically separated from their mothers
suffer dramatic alterations in both blood chemistry and brain
function.{8}
One major theory about why some people become
depressed and others do not holds that under conditions of
early trauma, a genetically based susceptibility to stress
creates a greater likelihood of intense stress-responses later
in life.{9}
This "vulnerability" is represented physiologically as actual
alterations in the brain. And because what is experiences as
"stressful" depends on one's subjective interpretation of
events, the brains in individuals with the same genetically
determined biology may respond differently. One may
demonstrate no brain changes; another may demonstrate very
significant changes.{10}
Thus the editor of Nature commented on the LeVay
research: "Plainly, the neural correlates of genetically
determined gender are plastic at a sufficiently early stage .
. .. Plastic structures in the hypothalamus [might] allow . .
. the consequences of early sexual arousal to be made
permanent."{11}
And of course all this presumes that the
research itself was of high quality. But two prominent
geneticists, Paul Billings and Jonathan Beckwith, writing in
Technology Review (published at the Massachusetts
Institute of Technology) write: LeVay "could not really be
certain about his subject's sexual preferences, since they
were dead." His "research design and subject sample did not
allow others to determine whether it was sexual behavior, drug
use, or disease history that was correlated with the observed
differences among the subjects' brains.{12}
LeVay's very method of defining homosexuality was very likely
to create inaccurate or inconsistent study groups."{13}
Because all human behavior is related in some
way to genes, we can nonetheless guess that one day higher
quality research will find genetic factors that correlate to
homosexuality. But remember, one ofthe fundamental principles
of research is that correlation does not necessarily imply
causation. With respect to whatever genetic or biological
factors are correlated to homosexuality we will need to be
very careful to understand what they mean and indeed, how
limited the implications really are.
To order: Send $17.50 ($15.00 plus $2.50 Shipping &
Handling).
Send cash, check or money order, prepaid. No
credit cards.
To: Hamewith Distributors P.O. Box 5191, Westport,
Connecticut 06881
Tel. 9am - 3pm EST (203) 221-0031, Fax
24H (203) 221-7937
Notes
{1}For
an excellent nontechnical critique of the limitations of
genetics research into many areas of human behavior, see
Horgan, J. (June 1993~. Eugenics Revisited, Scientific
American' pp. 12331.
{2}Ernulf,
K. E., Innala, S. M., and Whitam, F. L. (1989) Biological
Explanation, Psychological Explanation, and Tolerance of
Homosexuals: A Cross-National Analysis of Beliefs and
Attitudes, Psychological Reports 65, pp. 1003-10 (1 of
3).
{3}A
lower score on this scale means a less negative attitude
toward homosexuality.
{4}Piskur,
J., and Degelman, D. (1992) Effects of Reading a Summary of
Research about Biological Bases of Homosexual Orientation in
Attitudes Toward Homosexuals, Psychological Reports 71,
pp. 1219-25 (part 2 of 3).
{5}LeVay,
S. (1991). C`A Difference" In Hypothalamic Structure Between
Heterosexual and Homosexual Men, Science 253, pp.
1034-37.
{6}Swaab,
D. And Hofman, M. (1990~. An Enlarged Suprachiasmatic Nucleus
in Homosexual Men, Brain Research 537, pp. 141-48.
{7}Klivingston,
K. Assistant to the President of the Salk Institute, cited by
K. Lansing in Homosexuality: Theories of Causation,
Reorientation and the Politics and Ethics Involved,
Proceedings of the 1993 Annual Scientific Meeting of the
National Association for Research and Treatment of
Homosexuality, p. 50.
{8}Gabbard,
G. (1992). Psychodynamic Psychiatry in the "Decade of the
Brain," American Journal of Psychiatry 149, (8), pp.
991-98.
{9}Post,
R. (1992). Transduction of Psychosocial Stress into the
Neurobiology of Recurrent Affective Disorder, American
Journal of Psychiatry 148, (8), pp. 999-1010.
{10}Gabbard,
Psychodynamic Psychiatry.
{11}Maddox,
J. (September 1991). Is Homosexuality Hardwired? Nature
353, p. 13.
{12}Ibid.,
p. 60.
{13}Ibid.,
p. 61.
Photo credit: Robert Satter